Reading in the brain The science and evolution of a human invention

Stanislas Dehaene

Book - 2009

In this riveting investigation, Stanislas Dehaene provides an accessible account of the brain circuitry of reading and explores what he calls the "reading paradox": Our cortex is the product of millions of years of evolution in a world without writing, so how did it adapt to recognize words?

Saved in:

2nd Floor Show me where

418.4/Dehaene
0 / 1 copies available
Location Call Number   Status
2nd Floor 418.4/Dehaene Due Nov 29, 2024
Subjects
Published
New York. N.Y. : Viking 2009.
Language
English
Main Author
Stanislas Dehaene (-)
Physical Description
xi, 388 p. : ill., map ; 24 cm
Bibliography
Includes bibliographical references (p. 331-375) and index.
ISBN
9780670021109
  • Introduction The New Science of Reading
  • From Neurons to Education
  • Putting Neurons into Culture
  • The Mystery of the Reading Ape
  • Biological Unity and Cultural Diversity
  • A Reader's Guide
  • Chapter 1. How Do We Read?
  • The Eye: A. Poor Scanner
  • The Search for Invariants
  • Amplifying Differences
  • Every Word is a Tree
  • The Silent Voice
  • The Limits of Sound
  • The Hidden Logic of Our Spelling System
  • The Impossible Dream of Transparent Spelling
  • Two Routes for Reading
  • Mental Dictionaries
  • An Assembly of Daemons
  • Parallel Reading
  • Active Letter Decoding
  • Conspiracy and Competition in Reading
  • From Behavior to Brain Mechanisms
  • Chapter 2. The Brains Letterbox
  • Joseph-Jules Déjerine's Discovery
  • Pure Alexia
  • A Lesion Revealed
  • Modern Lesion Analysis
  • Decoding the Reading Brain
  • Reading is Universal
  • A Patchwork of Visual Preferences
  • How Fast Do We Read?
  • Electrodes in the Brain
  • Position Invariance
  • Subliminal Reading
  • How Culture Fashions the Brain
  • The Brains of Chinese Readers
  • Japanese and Its Two Scripts
  • Beyond the Letterbox
  • Sound and Meaning
  • From Spelling to Sound
  • Avenues to Meaning
  • A Cerebral Tidal Bore
  • Brain Limits on Cultural Diversity
  • Reading and Evolution
  • Chapter 3. The Reading Ape
  • Of Monkeys and Men
  • Neurons for Objects
  • Grandmother Cells
  • An Alphabet in the Monkey Brain
  • Proto-Letters
  • The Acquisition of Shape
  • The Learning Instinct
  • Neuronal Recycling
  • Birth of a Culture
  • Neurons for Reading
  • Bigram Neurons
  • A Neuronal Word Tree
  • How Many Neurons for Reading?
  • Simulating the Reader's Cortex
  • Cortical Biases That Shape Reading
  • Chapter 4. Inventing Reading
  • The Universal Features of Writing Systems
  • A Golden Section for Writing Systems
  • Artificial Signs and Natural Shapes
  • Prehistoric Precursors of Writing
  • From Counting to Writing
  • The Limits of Pictography
  • The Alphabet: A Great Leap Forward
  • Vowels: The Mothers of Reading
  • Chapter 5. Learning to Read
  • The Birth of a Future Reader
  • Three Steps for Reading
  • Becoming Aware of Phonemes
  • Graphemes and Phonemes: A Chicken and Egg Problem
  • The Orthographic Stage
  • The Brain of a Young Reader
  • The Illiterate Brain
  • What Does Reading Make Us Lose?
  • When Letters Have Colors
  • From Neuroscience to Education
  • Reading Wars
  • The Myth of Whole-Word Reading
  • The Inefficiency of the Whole-Language Approach
  • A Few Suggestions for Educators
  • Chapter 6. The Dyslexic Brain
  • What Is Dyslexia?
  • Phonological Trouble
  • The Biological Unity of Dyslexia
  • A Prime Suspect: The Left Temporal Lobe
  • Neuronal Migrations
  • The Dyslexic Mouse
  • The Genetics of Dyslexia
  • Overcoming Dyslexia
  • Chapter 7. Reading and Symmetry
  • When Animals Mix Left and Right
  • Evolution and Symmetry
  • Symmetry Perception and Brain Symmetry
  • Dr. Orton's Modern Followers
  • The Pros and Cons of a Symmetrical Brain
  • Single-Neuron Symmetry
  • Symmetrical Connections
  • Dormant Symmetry
  • Breaking the Mirror
  • Broken Symmetry ... or Hidden Symmetry?
  • Symmetry, Reading, and Neuronal Recycling
  • A Surprising Case of Mirror Dyslexia
  • Chapter 8. Toward a Culture of Neurons
  • Resolving the Reading Paradox
  • The Universality of Cultural Forms
  • Neuronal Recycling and Cerebral Modules
  • Toward a List of Cultural Invariants
  • Why Are We the Only Cultural Species?
  • Uniquely Human Plasticity?
  • Reading Other Minds
  • A Global Neuronal Workspace
  • Conclusion The Future of Reading
  • Acknowledgments
  • Notes
  • Bibliography
  • Index
  • Figure Credits
Review by Choice Review

Dehaene (College de France, Paris) uses neuroscience to trace the strictly human behavior of reading. Using brain research accomplished from the 1980s onward, the author shows how the human nervous system evolved from performing visual searches for food to scanning printed symbols for meaning. Dehaene focuses in particular on what he terms the "brain's letterbox," which scientists claim is universal in accommodating all symbols used in world languages. Dehaene applies these findings to the practical problem of teaching reading and treating reading problems such as dyslexia. In doing so, he calls for an alliance between education and cognitive science; cognitive science could provide research on how the brain functions when a person is reading, and education would provide the format in which to apply the research, helping humans learn to read or repair reading difficulties that already exist. (Dehaene supports the use of the phonics approach in accomplishing these tasks.) Conclusions that neural circuits in the human brain can be recycled for reading should help support the development of cognitive-based reading software, bringing hope to those for whom reading is an ordeal. An intriguing discussion but not one for inexperienced readers. Summing Up: Recommended. Researchers and professionals. T. R. Bitner DePauw University

Copyright American Library Association, used with permission.
Review by New York Times Review

AT this very moment, you are actually moving your eyes over a white page dotted with black marks. Yet you feel that you are simply lost in the universe of The New York Times Book Review, alert to the seductive perfume of a promising new novel and the acrid bite of a vicious critical attack. That transformation from arbitrary marks to vivid experience is one of the great mysteries of the human mind. It's especially mysterious because reading is a relatively recent invention, dating to some 5,000 to 10,000 years ago. Our brains didn't evolve to read. Stanislas Dehaene, a distinguished French cognitive scientist, has helped unravel that mystery. His gifts, on display in "Reading in the Brain," include an aptitude for complex experiments and an appetite for detail. This makes for excellent science but not, paradoxically, easy reading. Still, his book will repay careful study, even if it doesn't inspire blissful absorption. Dehaene begins by describing the remarkably complicated neural circuitry devoted to getting from marks to thoughts. He then explains how reading developed historically (from Sumerian inscriptions and Egyptian hieroglyphics to the Greek and Roman alphabets and Chinese characters), how we learn to read as children and why dyslexia makes reading so hard. Every time you complete a word recognition security test on a Web site, you are paying unconscious homage to the sophistication and subtlety of the reading brain. The most advanced spambots can't even recognize letters as well as we can, let alone recover the meaning that lurks behind them. Cognitive science has shown that the simplest experiences - talking, seeing, remembering - are the result of fiendishly complex computations. Dehaene's work, along with that of others, adds reading to the list. But Dehaene also makes an argument that goes beyond reading, an argument about human nature itself. In "Reading in the Brain," he adopts the rhetoric of innateness, a complex of ideas developed by Noam Chomsky 50 years ago and popularized by evolutionary psychologists like Steven Pinker. He argues that reading is highly constrained by fixed, innate brain structures with only a little flexibility, just enough to allow this unprecedented skill to emerge at all. But there are two very different kinds of innateness. Chomsky proposed that we are born with specific, genetically determined neural and cognitive structures, structures that go far beyond a few general learning mechanisms. This kind of innateness has become the established wisdom in cognitive science. The brain is not a blank slate. However, the other, more significant, kind of innateness concerns not the history of the mind but its future. Chomsky also argued that innate structure places very strong constraints on the human mind. Evolutionary psychologists who echo Chomsky say we are stuck with the same brains as our hunter-gatherer ancestors, with just a little tinkering around the edges. Many social scientists reject this second claim. A new generation of cognitive scientists and neuroscientists are starting to reject it, too. In the past few years, computer scientists have developed new machine learning techniques that allow computers to make genuinely new discoveries, and cognitive scientists have begun to discover that even young children's minds learn in much the same way. At the same time, neuroscientists have discovered that the brain is much more plastic - more influenced by experience - than we used to think. The brain is highly structured, but it is also extremely flexible. It's not a blank slate, but it isn't written in stone either. Dehaene describes some fascinating and convincing evidence for the first kind of innateness. In one of the most interesting chapters, he argues that the shapes we use to make written letters mirror the shapes that primates use to recognize objects. After all, I could use any arbitrary squiggle to encode the sound at the start of "Tree" instead of a T. But actually the shapes of written symbols are strikingly similar across many languages. It turns out that T shapes are important to monkeys, too. When a monkey sees a T shape in the world, it is very likely to indicate the edge of an object - something the monkey can grab and maybe even eat. A particular area of its brain pays special attention to those significant shapes. Human brains use the same area to process letters. Dehaene makes a compelling case that these brain areas have been "recycled" for reading. "We did not invent most of our letter shapes," he writes. "They lay dormant in our brains for millions of years, and were merely rediscovered when our species invented writing and the alphabet." However, the very fact that our brains have become so exquisitely adapted for reading looks like an argument against the second kind of innateness - the written in stone kind. Dehaene also endorses the Chomskyan view that reading is highly constrained - that "new cultural inventions can only be acquired insofar as they fit the constraints of our brain architecture" - but it's not so clear that he really believes it himself. For example, he argues that the primate brain has evolved to treat symmetrical shapes, like the letter pairs p and q, or b and d, as if they were the same. This explains why children, and dyslexics, have so much trouble distinguishing these letters. It also explains our extraordinary ability to "mirror-read" and "mirror-write." Many children spontaneously reverse not just single letters but whole paragraphs of text. But if reading is so tightly constrained by innate brain structure, we'd expect that we would simply never use letters like b and d at all. Instead, Dehaene shows how the reading brain has developed a new ability to discriminate these symmetries, even at the neural level. A developing brain that is exposed to symmetrical letters with different meanings will rewire and overcome its natural symmetry-blindness. WE are born with a highly structured brain. But those brains are also transformed by our experiences, especially our early experiences. More than any other animal, we humans constantly reshape our environment. We also have an exceptionally long childhood and especially plastic young brains. Each new generation of children grows up in the new environment its parents have created, and each generation of brains becomes wired in a different way. The human mind can change radically in just a few generations. These changes are especially vivid for 21st-century readers. At this very moment, if you are under 30, you are much more likely to be moving your eyes across a screen than a page. And you may be simultaneously clicking a hyperlink to the last "Colbert Report," I.M.-ing with friends and Skyping with your sweetheart. We are seeing a new generation of plastic baby brains reshaped by the new digital environment. Boomer hippies listened to Pink Floyd as they struggled to create interactive computer graphics. Their Generation Y children grew up with those graphics as second nature, as much a part of their early experience as language or print. There is every reason to think that their brains will be as strikingly different as the reading brain is from the illiterate one. Should this inspire grief, or hope? Socrates feared that reading would undermine interactive dialogue. And, of course, he was right, reading is different from talking. The ancient media of speech and song and theater were radically reshaped by writing, though they were never entirely supplanted, a comfort perhaps to those of us who still thrill to the smell of a library. But the dance through time between old brains and new ones, parents and children, tradition and innovation, is itself a deep part of human nature, perhaps the deepest part. It has its tragic side. Orpheus watched the beloved dead slide irretrievably into the past. We parents have to watch our children glide irretrievably into a future we can never reach ourselves. But, surely, in the end, the story of the reading, learning, hyperlinking, endlessly rewiring brain is more hopeful than sad. Alison Gopnik is the author of "The Philosophical Baby: What Children's Minds Tell Us About Truth, Love, and the Meaning of Life."

Copyright (c) The New York Times Company [December 13, 2009]
Review by Booklist Review

In a work of most benefit to reading teachers, Dehaene, a leading neuroscientist based in France, presents the latest developments in his field that apply to children's acquisition of the skill. A preview: Dehaene criticizes the so-called whole-language method because, according to his research, the brain doesn't process complete words. The way it really works, well, that's the substance of his text, which is filled with images of high-tech brain scans. Dehaene explains that word processing begins in an area of the cortex he colloquially calls the letterbox. Neuroscientists believe that the letterbox evolved to visually define the outlines of objects, and it found a new application with the invention of writing thousands of years ago. Describing experiments on cognition, Dehaene pulls in sound, and a child's lexical memory, to arrive at an integrated, though by no means experimentally final, theory of how we read. His discussion of reading problems offers encouragement to sufferers and teachers alike as it illuminatingly informs reading teachers of the underlying science of what they are doing.--Taylor, Gilbert Copyright 2009 Booklist

From Booklist, Copyright (c) American Library Association. Used with permission.
Review by Publisher's Weekly Review

The transparent and automatic feat of reading comprehension disguises an intricate biological effort, ably analyzed in this fascinating study. Drawing on scads of brain-imaging studies, case histories of stroke victims and ingenious cognitive psychology experiments, cognitive neuroscientist Dehaene (The Number Sense) diagrams the neural machinery that translates marks on paper into language, sound and meaning. It's a complex and surprising circuitry, both specific, in that it is housed in parts of the cortex that perform specific processing tasks, and puzzlingly abstract. (The brain, Dehaene hypothesizes, registers words mainly as collections of pairs of letters.) The author proposes reading as an example of "neuronal recycling"-the recruitment of previously evolved neural circuits to accomplish cultural innovations-and uses this idea to explore how ancient scribes shaped writing systems around the brain's potential and limitations. (He likewise attacks modern "whole language" reading pedagogy as an unnatural imposition on a brain attuned to learning by phonics.) This lively, lucid treatise proves once again that Dehaene is one of our most gifted expositors of science; he makes the workings of the mind less mysterious, but no less miraculous. Illus. (Nov. 16) (c) Copyright PWxyz, LLC. All rights reserved

(c) Copyright PWxyz, LLC. All rights reserved
Review by Library Journal Review

What's behind the invention of reading? Well, for starters, brain plasticity, the evolution of neurocircuits capable of processing visual with audio information, and the expansion of the prefrontal cortex leading to a behavior described as consciousness. The evolutionary infusion of these elements along with a novel hijacking from their evolved use intersects with human culture and incites a revolution: a culture with texts and brains that read those texts. All this drives neuroscientist Dehaene's (experimental cognitive psychology, CollEge de France) thesis that the invention of reading has less to do with constructs, such as alphabets, words, and sentence structures, than the mechanics and limits of our brains. Simply, our brains didn't evolve to read, but they are flexible enough to learn new tricks. Dehaene supports his thesis with references to a smorgasbord of research, traversing such subjects as anatomy, reading mechanics, primate evolution, history of linguistics, literacy, dyslexia, and brain symmetry. VERDICT This will appeal to a broad audience interested in the cognitive sciences, reading, and linguistics. Some chapters will attract those who teach reading and languages and parents of children with reading disabilities.-Scott Vieira, Johnson Cty. Lib., KS (c) Copyright 2010. Library Journals LLC, a wholly owned subsidiary of Media Source, Inc. No redistribution permitted.

(c) Copyright Library Journals LLC, a wholly owned subsidiary of Media Source, Inc. No redistribution permitted.
Review by Kirkus Book Review

A neuroscientist explains how the brain deals with reading. Dehaene (Experimental Cognitive Psychology/Coll'ge de France; The Cognitive Neuroscience of Consciousness, 2002, etc.) begins by pointing out that the brain contains circuitry exquisitely attuned to reading. Humans began to read only 5,000 years ago, so eons of evolution could not have designed it. Since genes haven't evolved to enable us to read, writing systems have adapted to constraints in the human brain. The author describes experiments using dazzling, high-tech devices that image the brain while a subject reads. The retina sends everything we see to the extensive visual areas at the rear of the brain. An instant later, any written word, in any language, lights up a tiny area. Closer examination of this "letterbox area" reveals a smaller section sensitive only to simple lines and curves, an adjacent area that forms these into letters and another that recognizes words. This is the identical area and mechanism which animals use to recognize objects in their environment, so evolution has cleverly recycled existing brain circuits to handle reading. Dehaene stresses that these findings should help teach readingphonics trump the whole word method, which has no basis in brain physiologyand treat dyslexia, which is rare in "transparent" languages (i.e., where one letter equals one sound) like Italian but epidemic in English where irregular spelling makes it much harder for the brain to decode words. Dense with ideas and experiments, but richly rewarding for readers willing to put in the effort. Copyright Kirkus Reviews, used with permission.

Copyright (c) Kirkus Reviews, used with permission.