Willful blindness Why we ignore the obvious at our peril

Margaret Heffernan, 1955-

Book - 2011

Saved in:
This item has been withdrawn.

2nd Floor Show me where

153.7/Heffernan
All copies withdrawn
Location Call Number   Status
2nd Floor 153.7/Heffernan Withdrawn
Subjects
Published
New York : Walker Pub. Co c2011.
Language
English
Main Author
Margaret Heffernan, 1955- (-)
Physical Description
294 p. ; 25 cm
Bibliography
Includes bibliographical references and index.
ISBN
9780802719980
  • Introduction
  • 1. Affinity and Beyond
  • 2. Love Is Blind
  • 3. Dangerous Convictions
  • 4. The Limits of Your Mind
  • 5. The Ostrich Instruction
  • 6. Just Following Orders
  • 7. The Cult of Cultures
  • 8. Bystanders
  • 9. Out of Sight, Out of Mind
  • 10. De-Moralizing Work
  • 11. Cassandra
  • 12. See Better
  • Acknowledgments
  • Notes
  • Bibliography
  • Index
Review by Booklist Review

Why did so many executives and employees of Enron, BP, and subprime lenders pretend that questionable company practices were business as usual? Why do so many pretend not to notice all the signs of a cheating partner? Heffernan (The Naked Truth, 2004) explores all the ways we are hardwired to blind ourselves, at our own peril, to information that disturbs our fixed notions about our lives. She cites examples from popular culture (The Sopranos and Mad Men), history (the Hitler regime), finances (Bernie Madoff), religion (the Catholic Church sex-abuse scandals), and our personal lives (marital infidelity) to describe the breadth and depth of deliberate blindness to misdeeds large and small. She draws from research on brain functioning, sociology, and psychology for instance after instance of how we ignore danger and will ourselves not to know when knowledge would threaten the orderly patterns of our lives. Finally, she examines the social and political implications of our willful blindness and how we can overcome the urge to look the other way.--Bush, Vanessa Copyright 2010 Booklist

From Booklist, Copyright (c) American Library Association. Used with permission.
Review by Publisher's Weekly Review

A thoughtful and entertaining treatise on the seductiveness-and consequences-of ignoring what's right in front of our eyes, from former CEO and author Heffernan (The Naked Truth). We frequently ignore painful or frightening truths, subconsciously believing that denial can protect us, she argues, but our delusions make us ever more vulnerable, and whatever suffering we choose to ignore continues unabated. The author draws examples from the private-Bernie Madoff's family's blindness to his Ponzi scheme; a woman who married an alcoholic; another unable to see that her husband is sexually abusing her daughter-to the public: Alan Greenspan ignoring the housing bubble, a soldier working for Hitler. She gives us an insightful look into the psychology of denial and makes an ethical and pragmatic argument for engagement rather than deflection. Heffernan's cogent, riveting look at how we behave at our worst encourages us to strive for our best. (Mar.) (c) Copyright PWxyz, LLC. All rights reserved.

(c) Copyright PWxyz, LLC. All rights reserved

groups were asked to assess how much they thought they would like the people whose faces they'd seen if they were to meet them in the future. They were also asked how far they believed those people to be similar to themselves.   The students who had seen the same face for four weeks believed more strongly that these were people they would like in real life. They also believed (on no evidence except the photograph) that those faces belonged to people who were similar to themselves. In other words, the familiar faces-- with no supporting evidence-- felt nicer. Women responded to the experiment in exactly the same way as men. A similar experiment, using irregular octagons, generated the same pattern of responses. The familiar makes us feel secure and comfortable.   This even pertains when we go looking for emotional experiences, as when we listen to music. It can be hard fully to enjoy a new piece the first time you listen to it; only after repeated hearings does it become a favorite. Part of that may be because if you're trying out, say, Mahler's Eighth Symphony for the first time, there is a lot to take in: two orchestras, two choirs, and eight soloists over eighty minutes won't create an instant impression. And listening to music is a hugely complex cognitive exercise. Even the White Stripes' "Seven Nation Army" can take some getting used to. But once we've heard it a few times, we're used to it and like it. And then we don't want something different. We want more of the same.   "We score hundreds of attributes of every song," says Tim Westergren, the founder of Pandora Internet Radio. "And then we find the matches between those songs-- and then that's what we recommend to you. Because we know that if you liked one piece of music, you are very, very likely to like another one that shares the same characteristics." Westergren's business does for music what eHarmony does for dating. Each song is scored manually by musicians for some four hundred attributes; there are thirty for the voice alone, capturing everything from timbre to layers of the voice to vibrato. Then that "score" is matched to other songs that have scores that are as closely similar as possible. Pandora software is doing to music what we do when we meet people: looking for matches. And, when it finds them, people feel very happy. "God, I love Pandora!" said Joe Clayton, a music fan in Boston. "I love it. I'm always finding new bands, new stuff that I just couldn't find otherwise-- certainly not in any music store. And it's kinda creepy-- but in a good way-- because they almost never give me something I don't like. Almost never."   More than fifty million people use Pandora, and many are avid evangelists. But what Pandora can't do is come up with that serendipitous suggestion that introduces you to something completely different from anything you've ever heard before. I like Bruce Springsteen, Frank Zappa, and the White Stripes-- but I also adore Handel. And given my first three preferences, Pandora wouldn't ever offer me Handel. Westergren acknowledges that limitation. "We're never going to take you from rock and roll to baroque music. Pandora is about broadening your selection-- but narrowing your taste. If you like jazz, you like more jazz. If you like hip- hop, you like more hip- hop. But Pandora is never going to take you from Springsteen to Handel."   All personalization software, whether eHarmony, Pandora, Amazon's book recommendations, or MyShape .com's clothing suggestions, does the same thing: makes our lives easier by reducing overwhelming choice. And software is doing it the same way that our brain does, by searching for matches. It's as though, online and off - line, our life is one gigantic game of Snap! This is immensely efficient: It means that the brain can take shortcuts because it is working with what it already knows, not having to start from scratch. When we find what we like, part of our plea sure is the joy of recognition.   But the flip side of that satisfaction is that we are rejecting a lot along the way. As Westergren says, we are narrowing our taste, reducing the music or books or clothes or people that might widen our horizons. Our brains aren't designed to draw us into experiences that are wild and different; there would be no advantage in doing something so risky. And so, by focusing in one direction and excluding others, we become blind to the experiences that don't match.   This is not to say that strange, serendipitous things never flow into our lives. Of course they do. You meet someone at work who introduces you to Handel and you develop a love of baroque music. Or-- more likely-- your son introduces you to Rammstein. But these encounters are random and risky. Remember Robert's problem with Albanian women.   There's a circle here: We like ourselves, not least because we are known and familiar to ourselves. So we like people similar to us-- or that we just imagine might have some attributes in common with us. They feel familiar too, and safe. And those feelings of familiarity and security make us like ourselves more because we aren't anxious. We belong. Our self- esteem rises. We feel happy. Human beings want to feel good about themselves and to feel safe, and being surrounded by familiarity and similarity satisfies those needs very efficiently. The problem with this is that everything outside that warm, safe circle is our blind spot.   Because not only are we rejecting music that doesn't match; we use these same processes to make important decisions in our everyday lives. When I had my first opportunity, as a producer at the BBC, to choose my own team, I hoped to hire people who would challenge me and each other and who would invest the entire project with intellectual richness and vigor. With all that firmly in mind, I selected liberal arts graduates who were all female, spoke several languages, and had birthdays within the same week in June. In other words, they were all like me.   Did I consciously intend to do that? Of course not. Like hiring managers the world over, I intended to hire only the best and the brightest and that's what I thought I was looking for. But did I also want people I'd feel comfortable working with, enjoy spending late hours with, people who shared the values of the project? Well, yes. I was biased, in favor of those just like me. Everyone is biased. But just as we are affronted when told that we're likely to marry and associate with those very similar to ourselves, so most people vehemently reject the idea that they are biased: others may be, but not us. "And why beholdest thou the mote that is in thy brother's eye but considerest not the beam that is in thine own eye?" is how the Bible puts it. Of course we consider the people who disagree with us to be the most biased of all.   It's recently become easier to identify and mea sure biases with a suite of tests called the Implicit Association Test, or IAT for short. Designed by three psychologists, the computer- based tests examine thoughts and feelings that exist outside of our conscious awareness or control.9 These may pertain to gender, age, race, or religion. In the test, participants are given two sets of images and two lists of words-- one positive, one negative. Images and words appear randomly on the screen and you have to associate them with positives or negatives. You may link "male" with "intelligent" or "old" with "stupid." When making a link that isn't comfortable for us, we take longer. And that delay, say the researchers, is telling: it takes more time to overcome bias. The longer we take to accept a match, the greater our bias.   Since 1998, more than 4.5 million people have taken these tests and the researchers have found that bias is pervasive among all of us, whether we think we're biased or not. White physicians are friendlier toward anonymous white patients than toward black ones. Seventy percent of citizens in thirty- four countries associate science more with men than with women. More than 80 percent of us have a bias against the elderly. Ordinary people (including the researchers who direct the project) harbor negative associations in relation to various social groups even though they say they don't and often wish, quite earnestly, not to. We see this play out in daily life everywhere. Go into any major corporation and look around. Despite de cades of diversity campaigns and millions of dollars invested in programs to make recruitment and retention less biased and more equitable, the homogeneity of most companies is overwhelming. Look at the lists of where the graduates of Harvard Business School or Wharton go and you will see the same phenomenon: armies marching into banks, financial institutions, consulting firms, year after year.   This is one reason why, despite a great deal of goodwill and commitment and even, in some countries, equality legislation, it has proved so hard to shift women into top roles, shovel venture capital into ethnic businesses, or train a lot of male midwives. It isn't the only reason, of course, but the fact that we like people like ourselves, are unconsciously biased in their favor, makes a big impact. Ste reo types are energy-saving devices; they let us make shortcuts that feel just fine. That's why they're so persistent.   The famous development of blind auditions for new symphony members provided graphic illustration of this point. Harvard economist Claudia Goldin and Prince ton's Cecilia Rouse found that when musicians were allowed to audition behind screens, where their gender could not influence the evaluation of their music, women's chances of making it through the first round increased by 50 percent-- and in the final rounds by 300 percent. Blind auditions have now become standard in the United States, with the result that the number of female players in major orchestras has increased from 5 percent to 36 percent.   In Europe, that practice is not ubiquitous-- and many musicians argue that this perpetuates both gender and ethnic discrimination. Some of this bias isn't even implicit: The Vienna Philharmonic did not accept women until twelve years ago and even now only 2 percent of its 137 musicians are women. Moreover, despite the recent rise to prominence of many brilliant Asian musicians, the Philharmonic has never hired a visible "non- Aryan" because it feels that such individuals would "destroy the ensemble's image of Austrian authenticity." Even white male musicians say that white male musicians prefer white male musicians. Music may be a universal language but not, apparently, universally.   The many voices arguing in favor of diversity in recent years have not been motivated only, or even primarily, by notions of social justice. The argument for diversity is that if you bring together lots of different kinds of people, with a wide range of education and experience, they can identify more solutions, see more alternatives to problems, than any single person or homogenous group ever could. Groups have the potential, in other words, to be smarter than individuals; that's the case put forward so compellingly by James Surowiecki in his book, The Wisdom of Crowds . But the problem is that, as our biases keep informing whom we hire and promote, we weed out that diversity and are left with skyscrapers full of people pretty much the same.   Just as we choose to work with people very like ourselves, we choose to live among them also. The psychologist David Myers says that the way we move around and build neighborhoods mirrors the way we choose our spouses.   "Mobility enables the sociological equivalent of 'assortative mating,' " he says. Now that we enjoy so much freedom to move around, choosing the jobs that we like, we can also choose the communities that we like. And by and large we choose "those places and people that are comfortably akin to ourselves."   Across the United States, the journalist Bill Bishop studied this pattern and found that over the last thirty years, most Americans had been engaged in moving toward more homogenous ways of living, "clustering in communities of like- mindedness." He calls this "the big sort" and what strikes him is how well defended these communities can be. When a lone Republican neighbor, living in a staunchly Democratic part of Austin, Texas, dared to articulate his political opinion in a local LISTSERV, the response he got was unambiguous: "I'm really not interested [in] being surprised by right- wing e-mail in my in box, no matter what its guise. It makes me feel bad, and I don't like it."15 As we have either more freedom or less security in our work, we move more. In 2008, thirty-four million people-- 11.9 percent of the population-- moved, and that was the lowest rate of migration since 1959. And when people move, they mostly choose to live among people who like the same coffee shops, bookstores, festivals, and politics. When Bill Bishop went on to map this trend across the United States, his data revealed that the preference people have for living with others like themselves made a clear impact on the political map of the country. Whereas in 1976, only 26 percent of people lived in landslide counties, by 2000 45.3 percent did. That didn't make for a lot of argument outside the polling stations.    "The neighborhood where we live," Rebecca says, "is lovely. The neighbors are lovely. The family on the other side of us, Paul and Juliet-- they are us! Mid- thirties, two boys. Slightly younger but exactly parallel. Juliet doesn't work, but I'm only part- time. The street is full of people like us. Each house is a carbon copy of the same kinds of people."   When Rebecca and Robert were house- hunting, she recalls that they were driven by price, proximity to work, and choice of schools. All her neighbors were using the same parameters, so perhaps it isn't so surprising that they'd all end up together.   "I guess you could say that," Rebecca concedes. "But there were other places that would have fit the bill. But when we came here, it was more than just functionality. We liked it here-- we still like it here. It feels right for us."   Bishop argues that we have come to demand living arrangements that won't challenge us. We seek confirmation and validation from those around us, even if it is just a matter of our pastimes. Wesleyan College in Connecticut caters to this desire, by offering twenty- eight different dorms organized around themes, including one for "eclectic" students. (Apparently even dislike of themes is a theme.) Colgate College in New York has a dorm for the lovers of foreign films. Affinity is a big draw.   These dorms are merely mirroring what is happening in the communities from which these students come; real estate developers bank on it. They design neighborhoods such as Covenant Hills in Orange County for Christians so it boasts a Christian school. The more exotically named Terramor, on the other hand, aims to be one of the largest environmentally friendly developments in America, marketed to "cultural creatives" who want to send their children to its Montessori school. As nice as they may be, what the dorms and developments also do is narrow the field of vision, providing environments in which the inhabitants can reside safe from challenge.   However, living in communities that look different may not deliver real social diversity. Years ago, my family and I lived in the North End of Boston, the colorful Italian district of the city. After living in Europe, we'd chosen it because we were used to, and enjoyed, hearing Italian spoken on the streets and liked the aroma of garlic in the evenings. But we didn't have any Italian friends. We had an Italian longshoreman as a neighbor but we knew Louis only well enough to say "ciao." Who were the people we spent time with? People like us: educated white professionals with young kids.   Living in London wasn't so different. Yes, we lived in Stockwell, which real estate agents would call "a mixed area"-- meaning an ethnically diverse and sometimes volatile combination of elegant early Victorian terraces and 1960s public housing. But you didn't see much traffic between those two architectures, not a lot of cups of sugar being borrowed. Teachers, professors, businesspeople, and TV producers lived in elegant stucco homes; single, often teenage parents lived across from them. At least we could see each other but that was about as far as it ever went.   By following our instincts to cluster together in like- minded communities, we reduce our exposure to different people, values, and experiences. But we also slowly but surely focus on what we know, losing sight of everything else. We may have more choices than ever before but our tastes are more narrow than ever, too.   Media companies understand this perfectly. They know that when we buy a newspaper or a magazine, we aren't looking for a fight. The Fox News fan does not buy the New York Times . My Italian neighbor might read the Boston Herald but I would never see the Boston Globe on his doorstep. We select our media knowingly, rejecting the programs, newspapers, and TV stations that we don't agree with because we feel comfortable sticking to the same groove. The search for what is familiar and comfortable underlies our media consumption habits in just the same way as it makes us yearn for Mom's mac 'n' cheese.   This is natural but it isn't neutral. In what he calls the "group polarization effect," legal scholar Cass Sunstein found that when groups of like- minded people get together, they make each other's views more extreme.18 (It is worth noting that Sunstein, a professor at Harvard, is married to Samantha Power, also a professor at Harvard, and they both are currently on leave serving the Obama administration. Even people who write about this behavior can't escape it.) Just as Pandora narrows your taste, like- minded people have the same impact on your opinions.   In 2005, Sunstein got together with some of his colleagues to conduct an experiment. They brought together two groups of like- minded people: liberals from Boulder, Colorado, and conservatives from Colorado Springs. In their respective groups, each was asked to deliberate on three topics: civil unions, affirmative action, and climate change. But before the discussions began, individual participants recorded their private opinions on each topic. Then the groups were mixed up and encouraged to discuss their views.   The group deliberations were consistently respectful, engaged, and substantive, but when they were finished, almost every member ended up with more extreme positions than they had held at the start. Conservatives from Colorado Springs who had been neutral on a climate- change treaty now opposed it. Boulder liberals who had felt somewhat positive about civil unions became firmly convinced of their merit. What small diversity each group might have had at the outset was, says Sunstein, "squelched," while the rift between them had grown larger.   Even when presented with a wide range of data and arguments, Sunstein's work with groups demonstrated that when individuals read, they focus on the information that supports their current opinion, paying less attention to information that challenges their views. Overall, people are about twice as likely to seek information that supports their own point of view as they are to consider an opposing idea.19 Rather than broadening their attitudes, the very process of discussion renders them blind to alternatives. We stop looking at places or jobs or information or people that will prove too uncomfortable, too tumultuous for our closely held beliefs. We may think we want to be challenged, but we really don't. Our intellectual homes are just as self- selected and exclusive as our physical homes.   In theory, the Internet was going to change all of this. Access to the world's store of knowledge was supposed to liberate us from the confines of the people we knew and the institutions to which we belonged. After all, online you can meet anyone from anywhere. But while it's true that all of us now have access to more information than ever before in history, for the most part we don't use it. Just like newspapers, we read the blogs that we agree with-- but there we encounter a virtually infinite echo chamber, as 85 percent of blogs link to other blogs with the same political inclination.   In fact, the great strength of the Internet lies in this ability to develop and connect affinity groups. It goes beyond connecting Tea Party members with other Tea Party members, and jihadists with jihadists. Wherever you live, what ever your age, whether you love orchids, aikido, or ideology, you can find and connect to like- minded enthusiasts. Why do you want to do that? Because in doing so, you gain access to shortcuts: information from people like you that you believe to be reliable. If you don't know how to lift and split your orchids, the community of orchid aficionados will save you a lot of money and grief. Off - line or online, what's the advantage of clustering in strongly defined communities of like- minded people? You believe the advice or recommendations your neighbors give you. If they like a school or a restaurant or a movie, well then you will probably like it too; you don't have to try them all or read all the reviews. We cling together because it feels comfortable and safe but also because it's highly efficient. We don't have to learn everything ourselves, the hard, slow way.    From the Hardcover edition. Excerpted from Willful Blindness: Why We Ignore the Obvious at Our Peril by Margaret Heffernan All rights reserved by the original copyright owners. Excerpts are provided for display purposes only and may not be reproduced, reprinted or distributed without the written permission of the publisher.